In a recent New Yorker article, the power of suggestion is examined and its results are somewhat startling to the average mind.
The power of suggestion is not new. In 1906, Hugo Münsterberg, the chair
of the psychology laboratory at Harvard University and the president of
the American Psychological Association, wrote in the Times Magazine about a false confession case. A woman had been found dead in Chicago, garroted with a copper wire and
left in a barnyard, and the simpleminded farmer’s son who had
discovered her body stood accused. The young man had an alibi, but after
questioning by police he admitted to the murder. He did not simply
confess, Münsterberg wrote; “he was quite willing to repeat his
confession again and again. Each time it became richer in detail.” The
young man’s account, he continued, was “absurd and contradictory,” a
clear instance of “the involuntary elaboration of a suggestion” from his
interrogators. Münsterberg cited the Salem witch trials, in which
similarly vulnerable people were coerced into self-incrimination. He
shared his opinion in a letter to a Chicago nerve specialist, which made
the local press. A week later, the farmer’s son was hanged.
It would take decades before the legal and
psychological communities began to understand how powerfully suggestion
can shape memory and, in turn, the course of justice. In the early
nineteen-nineties, American society was recuperating from another panic
over occult influence; Satanists had replaced witches. One case, the
McMartin Preschool trial, hinged on nine young victims’ memories of
molestation and ritual abuse—memories that they had supposedly forgotten
and then, after being interviewed, recovered. The case fell apart, in
1990, because the prosecution could produce no persuasive evidence of
the victims’ claims. A cognitive psychologist named Elizabeth Loftus,
who had consulted on the case, wondered whether the children’s memories
might have been fabricated—in Münsterberg’s formulation, involuntarily
elaborated—rather than actually recovered.
To
test her theory, Loftus gave a group of volunteers the rudimentary
outlines of a childhood experience: getting lost in a mall and being
rescued by a kindly adult. She told the subjects, falsely, that the
scenario was real and had taken place when they were young. (For
verisimilitude, Loftus asked their parents for biographical details that
she could plant in each story.) Then she debriefed the subjects twice,
with the interviews separated by one or two weeks. By the second
interview, six of the twenty-four test subjects had internalized the
story, weaving in sensory and emotional details of their own. Loftus and
other researchers have since used similar techniques to create false
memories of near-drownings, animal attacks, and encounters with Bugs
Bunny at Disneyland (impossible, since Bugs is a Warner Bros.
character).
Earlier this year, two forensic
psychologists—Julia Shaw, of the University of Bedfordshire, and Stephen
Porter, of the University of British Columbia—upped the ante. Writing
in the January issue of the journal Psychological Science, they described a false memory method,
not of getting lost in childhood but of committing a crime in
adolescence. They modeled their work on Loftus’s, sending
questionnaires to each of their participant’s parents to gather
background information. (Any past run-ins with the law would eliminate a
student from the study.) Then they divided the students into two groups
and told each a different kind of false story. One group was prompted
to remember an emotional event, such as getting attacked by a dog. The
other was prompted to remember a crime—an assault, for example—that led
to an encounter with the police. At no time during the experiments were
the participants allowed to communicate with their parents.
What
Shaw and Porter found astonished them. “We thought we’d have something
like a thirty-per-cent success rate, and we ended up having over
seventy,” Shaw told me. “We only had a handful of people who didn’t
believe us.” After three debriefing sessions, seventy-six per cent of
the students claimed to remember the false emotional event; nearly the
same amount—seventy per cent—remembered the fictional crime. Shaw and
Porter hadn’t put undue stress on the students; in fact, they had
treated them in a friendly way. All it took was a suggestion from an
authoritative source, and the subjects’ imaginations did the rest. The students seemed almost
eager to self-incriminate.
One young woman spun a
story about a kind of love triangle. In the first debriefing, she
remembered the incident as a fistfight between her and another girl. In
the second, she remembered having thrown a small rock at her adversary
after the girl uttered a slur. By the third debriefing, the rock had
grown to the size of her fist and she had hurled it at the girl’s face.
“It was very emotional,” Shaw said. “Each time she’d reënact the event,
the rock would fill her hand a little bit more.” Nothing in the woman’s
affect suggested that the memory was false. She earnestly believed in
the truth of her confession, as most of her fellow-participants did
theirs. The memory was vivid, loaded with details about the crime that
the interviewer had not furnished. Moreover, Shaw and Porter could find
no personality traits that distinguished the false confessors from the
few holdouts, and no way of identifying who was most susceptible.
Researchers conclude that these are troubling findings. They mimic, in the gentlest way, what can
happen during police questioning: a small lie, told to shake loose the
truth, rattles around in a suspect’s imagination and takes root. The
psychologist Saul Kassin has studied interrogation and false confession
for decades. He told me that Shaw and Porter’s experiment illustrates
perfectly how social pressure can make innocent people admit to
wrongdoing. “Think about the dilemma the suspect now faces: ‘I don’t
have a memory for this, but the person who took care of me does.
Therefore it must be true and I have to find a way to remember it.’ ”
After reading this article, this is another plausible premise to the LOST saga. As the plot started, we were on a fairly straight and narrow crash survival tale, but over the course of time, it moved off into very different, strange tangents unrelated to the initial premise. It is like minor suggestions were being conjured and amplified in a person's mind - - - creating a roller coaster of self-importance to criminal behavior memories. A theory is that LOST was the creation within some character's mind.